Humans are constantly at war with disease. We lob antibiotic missiles
at bacteria and toss vaccine-shaped grenades at viruses. We drop bombs
made of antibacterial soap and hand sanitiser on everything we can. The
battle between humans and parasites (an umbrella term that includes
viruses, bacteria and much larger creatures that thrive on a host) has
ancient roots, and exert as strong a force on evolution as predators,
drought or famine. Other species face similar threats, of course,
and we're reminded of the devastating effect parasites have in nature
every day. Tasmanian devils are suffering an epidemic of facial tumour
disease, a kind of parasitic cancer. A disease called chytridiomycosis,
which is caused by a particularly nasty fungus called Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis, has ravaged nearly a third of the world's frog populations. Up to 90% of koalas suffer from chlamydia, a sexually transmitted disease that's caused by bacteria called Chlamydia trachomatis, in some parts of Australia. Great apes like chimpanzees and gorillas and even humans are being hit hard by the Ebola virus, pneumonia and other diseases. It
makes a great deal of sense, then, that animals would have evolved
mechanisms for protecting themselves from infection and illness long
before we could simply swallow a few pills. So what defence mechanisms
have other species developed? Salmon living in close proximity to each other can quickly spread infectious lice (Getty Images) Young salmon call upon an army of helpers. Juvenile salmon
aren't strong enough to compensate for the salmon louse or sea louse,
and can die from infected wounds. This parasite is a type of small
crustacean called a copepod that floats around the sea with all the
other types of plankton. In 2007, the transfer of sea lice from wild
stocks caused an outbreak of a disease called infectious salmon anaemia among farmed Chilean salmon and trout. But
salmon and other piscine sufferers of sea lice interact symbiotically
with other, smaller fish species that eat the sea lice off their skin.
In the wild and in aquariums, it had long been known that fish like the
corkwing wrasse, ballan wrasse, cuckoo wrasse, goldsinny, and rock cook
have a role as cleaner fish. The cleaner fish get a nice meal, and the
salmon get a clean bill of health. In the early 1990s, Norway's Institute of Fisheries Technology Research discovered that they could control the prevalence of sea lice in salmon farms
by introducing those and other cleaner fish into the enclosures. One
experiment found that just one wrasse was sufficient to keep one hundred
salmon clean, and another found that a goldsinny could keep 150 salmon
in prime health. A single goldsinny could eat 45 lice from two salmon in
just 90 minutes. Elsewhere, the humble ant has also evolved some
unique social behaviours in order to keep their colonies healthy. In
some ways, ant colonies behave as if they're one large superorganism;
rather than leaving each individual responsible for its own health, the
colony has what researchers have called “social immunity”. Bury the dead
When a colony member dies, surviving ants go out of their way to remove
the lifeless body from the colony. It isn't yet known what sorts of
bacteria, viruses, or fungi grow on the bodies of dead ants, but
biologists have long thought that corpse removal was a behaviour that
evolved to keep the colony healthy, because the deceased individual
could have been, or could become, infected. But it had never been
proven, until now. Red ant colonies can quickly harbour infection – but they have ways of dealing with the problem (Science Photo Library) Earlier this year, Belgian researcher Lise Diez and colleagues finally found concrete evidence to bolster the hypothesis. The researchers maintained several colonies of common red ants, Myrmica rubra
in their laboratory for 50 days. Half of the colonies were free to
dispose of their corpses as they naturally would, but the others were
blocked from doing so. Starting from the eighth day, adult workers from
colonies that were permitted to remove corpses naturally were
significantly more likely to survive than adults from the restricted
ones. Impressively, the ants in the restricted colonies found
alternative mechanisms for reducing their exposure to corpses. The dead
ants were moved to the corners and the colony to reduce the number of
individuals who could pass near them, and especially to keep them away
from the developing larvae. Some restricted colonies also managed to
"bury" their dead under some cotton wool they had removed from
artificial water dispensers. Behaviours that reduce an animal's
exposure to infectious parasites, from hiring cleaner fish to hiding
dead corpses, probably evolved to combat the deadly pressure of disease,
just as animals evolved camouflage
to escape being gobbled up by toothy predators. A 2004 study of 60
ungulate species as the San Diego Zoo and Safari Park found that the
animals still automatically groomed themselves, despite the fact that
they were free from parasites. In other words, grooming evolved as a
kind of precautionary, preventative measure rather than as an an
immediate, reflexive response to skin irritation. Although the study
only looked at ungulates, it seems reasonable to assume that grooming
behaviours across the animal kingdom – from big cats licking themselves
to primates picking the parasites from their skin– may have evolved in a
similar way. Sometimes animal grooming behaviours closely resemble our
own. These snow monkeys are taking a bath in the hot springs of Japan
(Science Photo Library) "In studying and observing animals that live in relatively
clean laboratories, field stations and domestic environments, and that
are vaccinated against diseases and medically treated when sick,” wrote veterinary researcher Benjamin Hart in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews in 1988,
“it is easy to forget that animals evolved and thrived in environments
with an array of parasites long before human protective measures were
available." Hygiene issues We humans also
have an array of innate defenses against illness. By looking for
parasites in fossilised human faecal samples, in Egyptian mummies, and
in archaeological sites, and comparing them to parasites known to infect
humans and other apes, University of Cambridge researcher Piers
Mitchell identified a handful of parasites, like threadworms (nematodes that infect the intestines) or liver flukes
(flatworms that infect the liver or gall bladder), to which we may well
have our own innate physiological defenses, because we have had to
contend with them for a million years or more.
We should be more readily able to combat those infections than for
parasites to which we have only been recently introduced. At least,
that's the idea. Humans have battled with infectious gut worms for millennia, leading us to evolve our own defences (Science Photo Library) So what happens when you
throw everything you've got – antibiotics, hand sanitisers,
antibacterial soaps, vaccines, even radiation – to combat your own
illnesses and infections? For a time, human innovation has allowed us
the upper hand in the escalating "arms race" against the microscopic
beasts that make us sick. But now that we enjoy those "human protective
measures" we may ironically be making ourselves too clean. We could be wiping out beneficial, symbiotic microorganisms along with the harmful parasites. There
are some seemingly unpalatable ways being tested to deal with this.
Early trials suggest a host of allergies and autoimmune diseases could
be treated by swallowing parasitic worms, or by faecal transplants that re-introduce beneficial microbes back into the gut. While
we may think humans have the upper hand, we have to accept that
viruses, bacteria, and all the rest are crafty creatures themselves,
that have since evolved their own defenses against our tactics. No doubt
we will come up with more sophisticated ways in which to tackle the
threat. But one intriguing question is this: is there anything we can
learn from other animals' cleaning behaviours that could help us survive
our war with disease?
0 comments:
Post a Comment